A transient tingling sensation on my scalp, accompanied by an equally fleeting phosphene across my visual field, alerts me to the fact that 2 milliamps of direct current are now passing through my brain, the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) to be specific. No, I haven’t been denied extradition from a pro-electric chair state, I willingly made myself a component in the circuitry of a technology known as transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS).
It is widely accepted that everything we do has an effect on
the ‘wiring’ of our brains, a fact proposed most succinctly by
neuropsychologist Donald Hebb, whose words are forever paraphrased as “neurons
that fire together, wire together”. Hebb’s law is now known to rely on
long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD), the enhancement
and reduction of synaptic efficacy, respectively. These mechanisms of synaptic
plasticity are thought to be the fundamental processes which underlie learning
and memory, and perhaps even mood disorders and addiction. It is therefore of
little surprise that tDCS, a technology capable of modulating synaptic
plasticity, has become subject to a great deal of research in recent years.
TDCS involves the application of electrodes to the scalp
above particular regions of the brain, as determined by the Brodmann area map
used for electroencephalography (EEG); the regions stimulated dictate the
effects of the session. The anode exerts a depolarising influence on the
neuronal somata (neuronal cell bodies) of the cortex and hyperpolarises the
apical dendrites, whereas the cathode induces hyperpolarisation of the somata
and depolarisation of the apical dendrites. Relating this back to synaptic
plasticity, the areas affected by the anode become more likely to ‘fire’,
meaning their synapses are more prone to LTP, and, conversely, regions of the
brain affected by the cathode become less active and are more likely to undergo
LTD. This is more or less the extent to our understanding of the mechanism by
which the effects of tDCS are manifested.
The clinical applications of an electrical current applied
to the scalp have been known for years. As far back as 43 AD, in fact, Roman
emperor Claudius’ physician used the shocks of electric eels to abate the pain
of headaches! Today it is known that tDCS is capable of ameliorating a
multitude of pathological afflictions, from stroke damage to schizophrenia, but
also that you and I, as presumably healthy individuals, might derive benefit
from the occasional zap.
Attending to the former claim of therapeutic potential in
the ill, the montage (electrode placement) with which I am, to use the term
most loosely, experimenting today has
been found to remediate depression. Some studies have found that just 20
minutes of 2 mA anodal stimulation over the DLPFC to reduce self-reported
depression by as much as 10% for every week of daily use. Unfortunately, many
of the studies I have come across regarding tDCS are ‘open-label’, science
jargon denoting clinical trials in which both the researchers and participants
know which subjects are receiving which treatments (in this case, the real
treatment or a ‘sham’ control). Unlike its ‘double-blind’ antithesis,
open-label studies are plagued by the expectancy effects of both the
researcher’s overt enthusiasm, or lack thereof, for the treatment and the
subject’s expectations of its outcome. Consequently, one might denounce the
aforementioned results to be a direct outcome of the placebo effect. This
criticism has been largely dismissed by more recent double-blind trials and
studies investigating the relative efficacy of tDCS and established
pharmacological therapies such as sertraline (an SSRI antidepressant). Such
studies have found tDCS and SSRIs to be of equal efficacy, though a combination
of the two was found to be of superior efficacy to either alone, a synergistic
pairing I hope will soon be exploited in clinical practice.
For those who refrain from the use of recreational drugs due
to their deleterious effects or illegality, perhaps you might consider potentiating
your own endogenous substances for a similar effect? I recently came across a
most intriguing montage which achieves just that. With the anode attached to
the C3 Brodmann area, corresponding to the region of the scalp which lies above
the primary motor cortex of the left hemisphere, and the cathode pressed
against my upper right arm, effects reminiscent of those one might experience
following consumption of a weak opiate such as codeine were elicited almost
immediately. As someone who has done much experimenting, in the euphemistic
sense, this was a most welcome experience I quickly sought to investigate. A
google or two later and I found a publication released last year detailing the
analgesic potential of tDCS, an effect they put down to the ยต-opioid system.
Opioid receptors are those which transduce the effects of opiates and opioids
(substances of similar pharmacological profiles to opiates), like morphine and
methadone respectively. But the body possesses its own painkillers, including
enkephalins, endorphins and dynorphins to name but a few, and it is these
substances whose production is upregulated upon stimulation of the motor cortex.
What the paper failed to mention, however, was the euphoric sensations evoked
by this montage. Feeling like a character from Huxley’s Brave New World, I
amused myself with the idea of becoming a junky without ever having pierced a
vein or ‘chased the dragon’.
Another, more frequently studied, area of tDCS research
focuses on the technology’s potential as a cognitive
enhancer. The phrase is employed with ever increasing frequency as we
strive to match our efficiency with the demands of modern life, or perhaps to
simply mimic Bradley Cooper’s character in the film ‘Limitless’! Whilst I
shan’t delve too deeply into the ethical storm which stalks this phrase, I feel
that the practice must be discussed. The majority of research in this area
pertains to the augmentation of working memory, the ability to hold information
in one’s mind to permit its manipulation and analysis. Such research supports
the idea that anodal stimulation of the left prefrontal cortex, a brain region
implicated in a variety of executive functions, results in a significant
improvement in the working memory of healthy participants. However, some
experts admonish users of the trade-off between anodal excitation and cathodal
inhibition that is so integral to the device’s mechanism of action. By this,
they refer to the fact that whilst you might enhance the activity of one area,
with the anode, you will also suppress activity in the area beneath the
cathode. Minimising the impact of this trade-off is undoubtedly a task we must
prioritise in brain stimulation research, especially given the diffuse nature
of tDCS’ influence on the brain, which it seems may extend to subcortical
structures.
And so, whilst I must urge you to take caution, should you proceed to plug yourself into the mains (figuratively, a 9 volt battery is sufficient) tDCS is a wonderful medical development which, along with its successors: transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and high-resolution tDCS, I predict we will be seeing much more of in the coming years.
And so, whilst I must urge you to take caution, should you proceed to plug yourself into the mains (figuratively, a 9 volt battery is sufficient) tDCS is a wonderful medical development which, along with its successors: transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and high-resolution tDCS, I predict we will be seeing much more of in the coming years.